We are independent & ad-supported. We may earn a commission for purchases made through our links.

Advertiser Disclosure

Our website is an independent, advertising-supported platform. We provide our content free of charge to our readers, and to keep it that way, we rely on revenue generated through advertisements and affiliate partnerships. This means that when you click on certain links on our site and make a purchase, we may earn a commission. Learn more.

How We Make Money

We sustain our operations through affiliate commissions and advertising. If you click on an affiliate link and make a purchase, we may receive a commission from the merchant at no additional cost to you. We also display advertisements on our website, which help generate revenue to support our work and keep our content free for readers. Our editorial team operates independently from our advertising and affiliate partnerships to ensure that our content remains unbiased and focused on providing you with the best information and recommendations based on thorough research and honest evaluations. To remain transparent, we’ve provided a list of our current affiliate partners here.

What is the Philosophy of Science?

Michael Anissimov
By
Updated Jan 23, 2024
Our promise to you
LanguageHumanities is dedicated to creating trustworthy, high-quality content that always prioritizes transparency, integrity, and inclusivity above all else. Our ensure that our content creation and review process includes rigorous fact-checking, evidence-based, and continual updates to ensure accuracy and reliability.

Our Promise to you

Founded in 2002, our company has been a trusted resource for readers seeking informative and engaging content. Our dedication to quality remains unwavering—and will never change. We follow a strict editorial policy, ensuring that our content is authored by highly qualified professionals and edited by subject matter experts. This guarantees that everything we publish is objective, accurate, and trustworthy.

Over the years, we've refined our approach to cover a wide range of topics, providing readers with reliable and practical advice to enhance their knowledge and skills. That's why millions of readers turn to us each year. Join us in celebrating the joy of learning, guided by standards you can trust.

Editorial Standards

At LanguageHumanities, we are committed to creating content that you can trust. Our editorial process is designed to ensure that every piece of content we publish is accurate, reliable, and informative.

Our team of experienced writers and editors follows a strict set of guidelines to ensure the highest quality content. We conduct thorough research, fact-check all information, and rely on credible sources to back up our claims. Our content is reviewed by subject matter experts to ensure accuracy and clarity.

We believe in transparency and maintain editorial independence from our advertisers. Our team does not receive direct compensation from advertisers, allowing us to create unbiased content that prioritizes your interests.

Philosophy of science is what underlies science, what tells us how science should be done. Like any other domain of human knowledge, it evolves over time and can never be declared "perfect". Philosophy of science can be considered both a part of philosophy, because it is abstract and involves looking at the big picture in a certain way, and a part of science, because it incorporates experimental data about the effectiveness of different ways of doing and thinking about science. As such, it may be considered "meta-science" - science focused on science itself.

The scientific method, which is hundreds or perhaps even thousands of years old, was the first step on the road of stepping stones known as philosophy of science. The scientific method, among other things, consists of a sequence of key activities: observe, hypothesize, predict, experiment. This is a fine outline, but there is great disagreement on the details of each stage and where and how it is appropriate to apply them. Philosophy of science is meant to flesh out these details and test them empirically.

Many people have made important contributions to the philosophy of science. William of Ockham, a friar who lived in the 14th century, came up with what is today called Ockham's razor, which can be phrased in many ways, but the most popular is "entities should not be multiplied beyond necessity". Albert Einstein rephrased this as "make everything as simple as possible, but not simpler". Many centuries later, Ockham's razor was reformulated in a quantitative and mathematical way.

In the 1930s, Edward Sapir and Benjamin Whorf observed that different languages have different words for different objects, and the particular language we use biases the observations we make and the conclusions we draw from them. The genius of this observation was not really understood until the 70s, when many philosophers of science began to reference what became known as the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. It became obvious that so-called "Whorfianism" could be extended to all areas of the scientific process - as humans, our brain works a certain way, and every aspect of it has the potential to slightly bias our observations. Like there are optical illusions that prey upon our imperfect perceptual systems to produce gross errors in judgment, there are cognitive illusions that testably produce beliefs that contradict fundamental laws in probability theory.

W.V. Quine, the great philosopher-logician of the mid-20th century, argued that for any given set of empirical facts, a near-infinity of theories can be cooked up to explain them, given enough side notes and addendums. So we can never know which theory is "correct" until we get more data. Karl Popper rejected this extremism and replaced it with his theory of falsifiability - for something to be science, it must have the potential to be refuted by further experimentation. This proved to be one of the most frequently cited contributions to philosophy of science.

All these incremental improvements may sound substantial, but the largest contribution ever made to the philosophy of science comes from the Reverend Thomas Bayes, an 18th century minister, and his intellectual heir, E.T. Jaynes, who died in 1998. Building off of a result in probability theory called Bayes' rule, Jaynes formalized the process of hypothesis formation based on precise mathematical foundations. This kickstarted the school known today as Bayesianism, which has become very popular in the physical and computer sciences and continues to grow in acceptance. Bayes' rule accepts subjectivism - that we can never know anything 100%, but with varying degrees of confidence, which can be precisely updated based on incoming evidence and prior probabilities.

Today, philosophy of science continues to evolve, being contributed to by scientists and philosophers alike.

LanguageHumanities is dedicated to providing accurate and trustworthy information. We carefully select reputable sources and employ a rigorous fact-checking process to maintain the highest standards. To learn more about our commitment to accuracy, read our editorial process.
Michael Anissimov
By Michael Anissimov

Michael is a longtime LanguageHumanities contributor who specializes in topics relating to paleontology, physics, biology, astronomy, chemistry, and futurism. In addition to being an avid blogger, Michael is particularly passionate about stem cell research, regenerative medicine, and life extension therapies. He has also worked for the Methuselah Foundation, the Singularity Institute for Artificial Intelligence, and the Lifeboat Foundation.

Discussion Comments

By Illych — On May 03, 2011

The point about the particular language we use biasing our observations and the conclusions we draw from them calls to mind the famous double-slit experiment, one of the lynchpins of quantum mechanics. I'm not scientifically minded enough to be able to explain it simply and as correct as perhaps some other people (please correct me if I'm wrong!), but essentially the experiment was to determine whether light travels in particles or in waves. The end result was something far different. What eventually has been discovered over time that the way light travels depends on the methods used to observe it.

Just goes to show that our brains have a lot to do with how we perceive reality.

Michael Anissimov

Michael Anissimov

Michael is a longtime LanguageHumanities contributor who specializes in topics relating to paleontology, physics,...

Read more
LanguageHumanities, in your inbox

Our latest articles, guides, and more, delivered daily.

LanguageHumanities, in your inbox

Our latest articles, guides, and more, delivered daily.