We are independent & ad-supported. We may earn a commission for purchases made through our links.

Advertiser Disclosure

Our website is an independent, advertising-supported platform. We provide our content free of charge to our readers, and to keep it that way, we rely on revenue generated through advertisements and affiliate partnerships. This means that when you click on certain links on our site and make a purchase, we may earn a commission. Learn more.

How We Make Money

We sustain our operations through affiliate commissions and advertising. If you click on an affiliate link and make a purchase, we may receive a commission from the merchant at no additional cost to you. We also display advertisements on our website, which help generate revenue to support our work and keep our content free for readers. Our editorial team operates independently from our advertising and affiliate partnerships to ensure that our content remains unbiased and focused on providing you with the best information and recommendations based on thorough research and honest evaluations. To remain transparent, we’ve provided a list of our current affiliate partners here.

What Are the Different Types of Second Language Acquisition Theories?

By Meg Higa
Updated Jan 27, 2024
Our promise to you
LanguageHumanities is dedicated to creating trustworthy, high-quality content that always prioritizes transparency, integrity, and inclusivity above all else. Our ensure that our content creation and review process includes rigorous fact-checking, evidence-based, and continual updates to ensure accuracy and reliability.

Our Promise to you

Founded in 2002, our company has been a trusted resource for readers seeking informative and engaging content. Our dedication to quality remains unwavering—and will never change. We follow a strict editorial policy, ensuring that our content is authored by highly qualified professionals and edited by subject matter experts. This guarantees that everything we publish is objective, accurate, and trustworthy.

Over the years, we've refined our approach to cover a wide range of topics, providing readers with reliable and practical advice to enhance their knowledge and skills. That's why millions of readers turn to us each year. Join us in celebrating the joy of learning, guided by standards you can trust.

Editorial Standards

At LanguageHumanities, we are committed to creating content that you can trust. Our editorial process is designed to ensure that every piece of content we publish is accurate, reliable, and informative.

Our team of experienced writers and editors follows a strict set of guidelines to ensure the highest quality content. We conduct thorough research, fact-check all information, and rely on credible sources to back up our claims. Our content is reviewed by subject matter experts to ensure accuracy and clarity.

We believe in transparency and maintain editorial independence from our advertisers. Our team does not receive direct compensation from advertisers, allowing us to create unbiased content that prioritizes your interests.

Second language acquisition theories are distinctly different and distinguished from natural first language acquisition by children. There is no single accepted explanation of how people, particularly adults with mature cognitive skills, learn a second language. Several related disciplines — education, linguistics, psychology and neurology — have contributed theoretical ideas and research studies. The many theories and hypotheses also make a clear distinction between a multilingual person and the process of a person acquiring fluency in additional languages.

In general, most second language acquisition theories parse the task. There is a native first language, sometimes abbreviated L1; the second language to be learned is sometimes referred as the “target language” and abbreviated L2. As new vocabulary and rules of grammar and syntax are learned, they are initially retained in a “lexical memory” which is essentially a store of definitions and declarations. An “input” event in the second language is processed, perhaps compared to the known information in memory, to derive at new conclusions and interpretations. The resulting “output” provides either positive or negative feedback to refine both memory and process.

The greatest influence on second language acquisition theories is the idea proposed by Noam Chomsky, the social philosopher regarded as the “father of modern linguistics,” that all languages have a “universal grammar.” Learning a second language is as easy as habituating all the minor rules of grammar peculiar to the language. The second most significant influence on second language acquisition theories is the concept of an “interlanguage.” A second language learner develops an indefinable, but systematic, third language that mediates between the first and second. Awkward mistakes such as, “I goed shopping yesterday,” are believed to be derived from this interlanguage.

Understanding the acquisition of a second language is an abstract theoretical exercise, as even modern brain imaging technologies cannot definitively map the cognitive process. Most studies, and their theoretical conclusions, concern the linguistic characteristics of a second language input, and a learner’s resulting output. The learner is categorized for level of proficiency. Of particular interest is linguistic mistakes in their output. The mistakes are scientifically replicable and reveal something of the underlying processes which cause them.

A set of hypotheses collectively called the Monitor Theory posits in essence that a learner possesses evaluative filters for both input and output, as well as one for the accuracy or effectiveness between them. For example, the input must be at least at a learner’s comprehension level; otherwise the input does not pass through the filter. Additionally, particularly in the early stages of learning, people exercise a great deal of control and deliberation over their output.

Theories emphasizing input and output have direct application in teaching a second language. The Monitor Theory, for example, includes an affective component to the filters that inhibits both input and output when subjected to stress or anxiety. A second language school or course will strive to establish ease in a learner, to allow for mistakes in understanding, and replying to, an unfamiliar utterance.

There are several other significant second language acquisition theories and hypotheses. The role of memory — both short term and long term, both lexical such as vocabulary and procedural such as grammar — is clearly very important. Some theorists note that learning occurs at certain cognitive thresholds. The theory of processability, for example, suggests that only when an interlanguage has established a restructuring order of the native language can a second language be learned. Other theories attempt the ultimate ambition of blurring the distinction between acquisition and learning, so that learning a second language might someday become as effortless as acquiring a first language.

LanguageHumanities is dedicated to providing accurate and trustworthy information. We carefully select reputable sources and employ a rigorous fact-checking process to maintain the highest standards. To learn more about our commitment to accuracy, read our editorial process.

Discussion Comments

By serenesurface — On Feb 08, 2015

I don't think that learning a second language will ever be as effortless as learning a first language. Children seem to have an easier time with second language acquisition but it's still much more difficult than a first language.

By discographer — On Feb 07, 2015

These theories sound very clear and surprisingly pragmatic. But in reality, I don't really think it works this way. Someone may have the potential to learn a second language and may follow all of these steps for learning it, but may not succeed. I have personally experienced this. I actually think that there are many different factors going into language acquisition. Some very important factors are interest and persistence.

People who aren't truly interested in a language and do not persist in learning it for long periods, will learn very little and will probably forget it when the lessons end.

By literally45 — On Feb 07, 2015

I didn't know about this interlanguage phenomenon until recently. For years, I couldn't understand why my Indian friends used certain phrases. For example, a friend would often say "do this" in the beginning of sentences, or use "only" at the end of sentences. It seemed strange to me. But as I learn Hindi, I realize that this is actually just the English translation of common Hindi phrases and usages. Indians often say "aise karo" when giving friends advice and it translates into "do this" in English. So all those strange little phrases that people use in their second language can be explained with their first language! I find it very interesting.

LanguageHumanities, in your inbox

Our latest articles, guides, and more, delivered daily.

LanguageHumanities, in your inbox

Our latest articles, guides, and more, delivered daily.